ORIGINAL  
Niger J Paed 2013; 40 (3) : 270 –274  
Abdulkadir MB  
Johnson WBR  
Caregivers’ perceptions of childhood  
fever in Ilorin, North-Central  
Nigeria  
DOI:http://dx.doi.org/10.4314/njp.v40i3,13  
Accepted: 22nd January 2013  
Abstract Background: Fever  
remains a common clinical indica-  
tor of disease, accounting alone  
for over 25% of paediatric emer-  
gency rooms consultations. Per-  
ception of this important sign is a  
crucial prelude and determinant of  
outcome in febrile children. The  
aim was to determine knowledge  
and attitudes of parents regarding  
fever in their children.  
Methods: The study is a cross-  
sectional descriptive study carried  
out at the Emergency Paediatric  
Unit of the University of Ilorin  
Teaching Hospital (UITH). Four-  
hundred under-five children  
30.3% of caregivers could cor-  
rectly describe what fever was.  
The most frequently utilised fever  
detection method was tactile as-  
sessment. Only 3.2% of the care-  
givers used a thermometer to de-  
tect fever. Social class, maternal  
age and religion significantly in-  
fluenced the decision to use ther-  
mometers. The most common  
remedies caregivers would use for  
fever in their children were to give  
paracetamol (96.3%), sponging  
(73.2%) and a warm bath (63.8%).  
About 61% of caregivers had a  
wrong perception of the possible  
complications of fever.  
Abdulkadir MB  
Johnson WBR  
(
)
Department of Paediatrics and Child  
Health, University of Ilorin /University  
of Ilorin Teaching Hospital, Jebba  
Road, Ilorin, Kwara State.  
Email: docmohng@yahoo.com  
presenting with fever were  
Conclusions: Caregivers in this  
study had a poor knowledge of  
fever, and parental educational and  
socioeconomic status impacted on  
their responses to fever. There is a  
need for education of caregivers at  
all contacts with the healthcare  
system on fever, and its manage-  
ment.  
recruited along with their caregiv-  
ers. A semi-structured question-  
naire was administered to collect  
information on sociodemograph-  
ics, and caregiver’s knowledge  
and  
attitudes regarding fever in their  
wards.  
Results: The mean age of the  
caregivers was 29.5 ± 4.46 years  
Keywords: Antipyretic, Attitude,  
(
Range 21 – 41 years). Only  
Fever, Thermometry  
Introduction  
unnecessary, and potentially dangerous. This so called  
5
fever phobia” may result in parents taking potentially  
Fever represents a perception of an elevated tempera-  
ture. In clinical practice, the relevance of fever is that it  
is a sign of a current illness. It remains one of the most  
harmful actions, such as use of herbal concoctions,  
1
scarification, and excess-i1v1e use of antipyretics, amongst  
7
others. Several studies have attempted to determine  
frequently encountered paediatric problems, accou-n4ting  
the knowledge and perceptio7,n8 of parents regarding fe-  
ver. Some of these studies have implicated factors,  
such as the level of education, ethnic group, and socio-  
economic status, as possible determinants of parental  
knowledge and perception of fever. There have been few  
2
for 25% of visits to paediatric emergency rooms. In-  
deed, it is often the first symptom noted by parents, and  
frequently, the initial “signal” of illness in the child.  
Parents have been known to have fears of the harmful  
effects of fever in their children, and often see fever as  
1
1
published studies from Nigeria on the subject matter.  
the main5,p6roblem rather than a symptom of a variety of  
This relative gap in knowledge on this issue, in Nigeria  
and indeed the West African Sub-region forms the justi-  
fication for this study. The aim of the study is to deter-  
mine parental knowledge about fever and their percep-  
tion of it in their children.  
5
diseases.  
It was Schmitt that first referred to this  
parental fear about fever as “fever phobia”, in order to  
emphasize the high prevalence, as well as the unrealistic  
nature o5f these fears. These fears, as described by  
Schmitt, often lead to parents taking actions that are  
2
71  
Materials and Methods  
ratio of 1.5:1. All caregivers recruited were the mothers.  
The mean maternal age was 29.5 ± 4.46 years (Range 21  
– 41 years). Majority of the mothers [298(74.5%)] were  
of the Yoruba ethnic group. Other ethnic groups in-  
cluded Hausa (13.5%), Ibo (3.3%) and others (8.8%).  
Caregiver religion was Islam amongst 293(73.3%)  
mothers and Christianity in 107 caregivers (26.8%).  
Regarding educational status of the mothers, only 107  
(26.8%) had attained tertiary education. One hundred  
and twenty four (31.0%) mothers had secondary educa-  
tion; 92(23.0%) had acquired primary education; and 77  
(19.3%) had no formal education. Using the metho2d of  
This is a cross-sectional study in which the subjects are  
children aged between 2 months and 59 months, and  
their parents/caregivers. The study was carried out at the  
Emergency Paediatric Unit (EPU) of the University of  
Ilorin Teaching Hospital (UITH). The hospital is located  
in Ilorin, which is the capital city of Kwara State, situ-  
ated in the North Central geopolitical zone of Nigeria.  
Four hundred children/ caregiver pairs were recruited  
over a four month period. Ethical clearance was ob-  
tained from the University of Ilorin Teaching Hospital’s  
Ethical Review Committee. Individual informed consent  
was also obtained from the mother, father, or the care-  
giver of the child as applicable. Caregivers of children  
aged less than five years, admitted into the EPU with  
fever were recruited consecutively. Subject recruitment  
was done as early as possible after presentation, after  
institution of the required immediate care as determined  
by the clinical condition of the child. It was considered  
pertinent to exclude caregivers of children with critical  
illness from the study. Siblings of children who had al-  
ready been recruited into the study were also excluded.  
1
Social Classification recommended by Oyedeji, 38  
(9.5%) of mothers were categorised as social classes I,  
69(17.3%) as social class II, 112(28.0) as social class  
III, and 156(39.0%) and 25(6.3%) as social classes IV  
and V respectively. The mode of the social class of the  
children was IV, and the median III.  
In response to the open question, “what is fever”, only  
121 parents/ caregivers (30.3%) were able to correctly  
identify fever as an increase in body temperature above  
what is considered normal. The most commonly utilised  
method for detecting fever by respondents was by touch-  
ing the child; this was used by 390(97.5%) mothers  
(Table 1). Seventy-four percent of these used the back of  
their hands to “feel for fever” in their children, while the  
remaining 104(26%) used their palm. The parts of the  
body palpated (to assess for fever) by the respondents  
who used palpation were the head in 306(76.5%), and  
the abdomen in 182(45.6%). Other parts felt were the  
chest in 43.3% and the neck in 31.8% of respondents.  
Various combinations of parts were felt to assess for  
fever amongst those mentioned above, of which the  
most common was the head and chest (35.4%).  
A semi-structured study proforma was administered on  
all subjects recruited. Socio-demographic data compris-  
ing age and gender of the child, parental religion and  
ethnic group(s), as well as age, educational status and  
occupation of parents was obtained. Information on par-  
ent(s)/caregivers’ knowledge, and perceptions regarding  
fever were obtained The items used to determine knowl-  
edge and perception explored the spheres of: definition  
of fever; causes of fever; best means to detect fever;  
complications of fever; and management of fever, inclu-  
sive of their sources of knowledge. Others include steps  
that should be taken if a child has fever; current con-  
cerns regarding fever in your child; and use of antipyret-  
ics. For the purpose of the study, convulsions were con-  
sidered to be an appropriate concern in relation to fever.  
Others such as brain damage, dehydration, death, were  
considered inappropriate concerns. Data entry and  
analysis were carried out with a micro-computer using  
the Epi-Info version-6 software package.Frequency dis-  
tribution tables and cross-tabulation of variables were  
generated. Measures of central tendency and dispersion  
of quantitative variables, as well as proportion for quali-  
tative variables were also determined. A thematic ap-  
proach was used to analyse some qualitative variables.  
The chi-square and student-t tests were used to identify  
significant differences for categorical and continuous  
variables respectively. Mean, standard deviations, and  
range were provided as appropriate. Yates correction  
and Fisher’s exact tests were utilised as appropriate.  
A p-value of <0.05 was considered significant.  
Table 1: Distribution of respondents by method of fever  
detection  
Method of fever detection*  
Frequency(%)  
Touch  
390(97.5)  
19(4.8)  
47 (11.8)  
10(2.5)  
Thermometer  
Appearance**  
Others  
*Some mothers gave multiple responses  
**Appears weak, appears sick, rigors  
All caregivers that would use a thermometer to measure  
temperature belonged to social classes I-II, as compared  
to the group that would not use a thermometer to meas-  
ure temperature where 31.0% belonged to social class I-  
II (p <0.000001) (Table 2). The mean maternal age of  
caregivers that would use thermometers to measure their  
child’s temperature was significantly lower at 25.8 ±  
2
.11 years (p = 0.00018) as compared to the group that  
would not use thermometers which was 29.7 ± 4.47  
years (Table 2). About 48% of caregivers who would  
measure temperature with a thermometer were Muslims,  
while among those who would not use a thermometer to  
detect fever, 74.5% were Muslims (p = 0.0045)  
Results  
Caregivers of four-hundred under-five children aged 2-  
5
6 months were recruited. The mean age of the children  
(
Table 2).  
was 25.6 ± 16.8 months, with a Male: Female (M:F)  
2
72  
Table 2: Sociodemographic indices of mothers versus  
detection of fever by thermometer  
Regarding the attitude of caregivers to the use of  
paracetamol for fever, there was no significant differ-  
ence in mean maternal age (p = 0.647) and maternal  
educational status (p = 0.156) between caregivers that  
would use paracetamol and those that would not  
2
Variable  
Use a ther-  
mometer  
Not use a  
thermometer  
(n=381)  
χ
P
(n=19)  
Social class  
I - II  
III  
(
Table 4). All caregivers (100%) that would not use  
19(100%)  
0(0%)  
0(0%)  
88(23.1%)  
112(29.4%)  
181(47.5%)  
paracetamol were Muslims, as against 72.2% of those  
who would use paracetamol (Table 4). This difference  
was statistically significant (p = 0.0085).  
54.62  
54.62  
<0.00  
0001  
IV-V  
Educational status-  
None/ Islamic  
Primary/ Secondary  
Tertiary  
0(0.0%)  
0(0.0%)  
19(100.0%) 88(23.1%)  
77(20.2%)  
216(56.7%)  
0.000  
0001  
Table 4: Sociodemographic indices of caregivers versus  
attitude to use of paracetamol in febrile children  
2
Maternal age(years)  
Mean ± SD  
Variable  
Use  
paracetamol  
Would not  
use paraceta-  
mol (n=15)  
χ
p
25.8 ± 2.11  
29.7 ± 4.47  
t=3.7  
8
0.000  
18  
(n=385)  
Social class  
I - II  
III  
IV-V  
Educational status  
None/Islamic  
Caregiver religion  
Islam  
Christianity  
102  
112  
171  
5
0
10  
6.20  
0.0451  
9(47.4%)  
10(52.6%)  
284(74.5%)  
97(25.5%)  
6.819  
0.004  
5
77(20.0%)  
0(0.0%)  
10(66.7%)  
5(33.3%)  
Fig 1 shows that 360(90.0%) caregivers had obtained  
information about fever recognition from their parents,  
Primary/Secondary 206(53.5%)  
Tertiary  
3.716  
0.156  
102(26.5%)  
1
32(33.3%) obtained information from other relatives,  
Maternal age  
Mean ± SD  
29.46 ± 4.55  
30.0 ± 0.85  
t=0.45  
9
0.6466  
0.0085  
and 125(31.3%) obtained information from their friends.  
Others are as shown in Fig 1. Amongst mothers who  
would use a thermometer to measure temperature, 19  
Caregiver religion  
Islam  
278(72.2%)  
107(27.8%)  
15(100%)  
0(0%)  
5.69  
(
100%) of them had their source of information from  
Christianity  
doctors, as against 40(10.5%) of those who would not  
2
use a thermometer (χ = 115.3 p<0.0001).  
Table 5 shows the spectrum of major concerns of care-  
givers regarding the possible consequences of fever in  
their children. The most frequently identified concern  
was convulsion which was indicated by 331 (82.8%)  
caregivers, followed by death (43.0%); and brain dam-  
age (27.5%). There was a significant difference between  
the social classes and educational status of mothers who  
had appropriate concerns as compared to those whose  
caregivers had inappropriate concerns regarding conse-  
quences of fever (p = 0.0000001 and p=0.000151 re-  
spectively)(Table 5).  
Fig 1: Distribution of respondents by source of informa-  
tion on detecting fever.  
Parents  
Relatives  
Friends  
360  
132  
125  
Doctors  
59  
Nurses  
53  
Other Healthcare…  
School  
38  
38  
Table 5: Distribution of caregivers’ concerns regarding fever  
Consequence(s) of fever*  
Frequency (%)  
0
100 200 300 400  
Convulsions  
Dehydration  
Brain damage  
Death  
331(82.8)  
80(20.0)  
110(27.5)  
172(43.0)  
15(3.8)  
Frequency  
With regard to steps that caregivers would take if their  
children have fever, the most common steps that would  
be taken are to give paracetamol (385 mothers), and  
sponging with tepid water(289 mothers). Others are as  
shown in Table 3.  
No concerns  
*
Some mothers had more than one concern  
Table 6: Social class of mother versus concerns regarding  
childhood fever  
2
Variable  
Appropriate Inappropriate  
χ
p
Table 3: Distribution of caregivers regarding steps that should  
be taken for a febrile child  
concerns*  
concerns †  
(n=244)  
(n=156)  
Action taken  
Frequency(%)  
Social class  
I-II  
III  
63(40.4%)  
55(35.3%)  
38(24.3%)  
44(18.0%)  
57(23.4%)  
143(58.6%)  
Paracetamol  
Sponging with tepid water  
Warm bath  
Undressing  
Others**  
385(96.3)  
289(73.2)  
144(36.0)  
251(62.8)  
112(28.0)  
47.25  
17.6  
0.000  
0001  
IV-V  
Educational status  
None/ Islamic  
Primary/ Secondary  
Tertiary  
15  
88  
53  
62  
128  
54  
0.000  
151  
*
Some mothers used more than one method  
** Fanning, praying and the use of herbal concoctions  
2
73  
*
in combination, including respondents who identified convulsions in  
addition to any of the above  
Convulsions alone  
Other concerns, such as brain damage, dehydration, death, singly or  
of this tactile method of assessin15g fever has remained  
limited to that of a screening tool.  
Also, it is equally noteworthy that with the prevalence of  
7
4% in the present study the most commonly used site  
for tactile assessment of fever by respondents was the  
dorsum of the h16and. This is similar to the findings of  
Akinbami et al in Ibadan, where 77.1% preferred to  
use the dorsal surface of the hand to assess for fever.  
This may be accounted for by the similarity in the  
Ibadan and Ilorin study populations, which are predomi-  
nantly of the Yoruba ethnic group. Furthermore, an ad-  
ditional observation from the present study was the com-  
mon parental perception that the head and abdomen con-  
stituted the important parts of the body from which fever  
could be most reliably17detected. This is similar to the  
findings of Singh et al in India, where majority of the  
adult patien16ts felt the forehead to identify fever. Akin-  
bami et al reported slightly different findings as the  
head and neck were the most preferred parts felt in their  
study. It is interesting to observe that about 12% of  
Discussion  
Overall, the findings of the present study suggest that  
caregivers in the study population had a poor knowledge  
of fever. This ob1servation is consistent with the findings  
1
of Tagbo et al in Enugu. In that study none of the  
mothers could identify what temperature actually indi-  
cates fever. Though the current study did not examine if  
mothers could identify the correct temperature that con-  
stitutes fever, it is instructive to note that only 30.3% of  
caregivers could identify fever as an increase in body  
temperature above what is considered normal. Rather  
most of the responses obtained suggested that parents  
view fever as a disease, as against a symptom of many  
possible diseases. This probably relates to the traditional  
perception of fever, and the fact that words in some local  
dialects used to describe fever (Eg Zazzabi in Hausa) are  
used synonymously with “malaria”. It is evident from  
the current study that only a few parents recognised that  
the use of a thermometer was the best method for detect-  
ing fever. This observation is similar to the finding11s of  
respondents would use the “appearance of the  
child” (appears weak, appears sick, rigors) to detect the  
presence of fever in their children. This is similar to the  
9
finding of Al-Eissa in Egypt. With very few respon-  
dents answering in the affirmative, the present study  
shows that most of the caregivers who would use  
thermometers to measure temperature had received this  
information from doctors. The operational import of this  
finding is the apparent need to involve the lower level  
healthcare manpower (who are expected to have more  
frequent contact with parents) in the education of par-  
ents regarding the timely value of use of thermometers  
for detecting fever in their children. In the current study,  
socioeconomic status of caregivers appears to influence  
the decision to use thermometers to measure tempera-  
9
Al-Eissa et al in Saudi Arabia and Tagbo et al in  
Enugu. This13was however, in stark contrast to those of  
Blumenthal in the presumably much more enlightened  
British population, where about 73% of caregivers  
reportedly used thermometers for ident3ifying fever and  
1
rated them better than use of palpation. This difference  
may be explained by the obvious dissimilarity in the  
literacy level and the socioeconomic classes of caregiv-  
ers in the two study populations. In the current study,  
9
ture. This is not in keeping with the reports of Al-Eissa  
4
2.3% of mothers had no formal education or only pri-  
where there was no difference in the use of thermome-  
ters amongst parent of high and low socioeconomic  
classes. The statistically significant younger age of care-  
givers who would use a thermometer as against those  
who would not, may suggest a positive change in per-  
ceptions with the younger generation. This may also be  
a reflection of increasing education in the younger age  
group.  
mary education. Their educational status was compara-  
tively poorer than the study populations in Enugu (22%  
had no formal education or primary education) and  
Saudi Arabia (25.4% of mother3s had no formal educa-  
1
tion). In Blumenthal’s study, however, the authors  
deliberately left out the presumably “uneducated” moth-  
ers who could not read English. This is further supported  
by the finding in this study that families with higher  
socioeconomic status were more likely to use ther-  
mometers. The effect of a higher socioeconomic class on  
knowledge of fever was further demonstrated, in this  
study, by the relationship between social class and con-  
cerns regarding fever. This is similar to the findings of  
Concerns about fever and its potential harmful effects  
may lead to parental behaviour that may be harmful to  
the child’s health, such as use of herbal concoctions,  
scarification, and covering the child, or indeed under-  
mine a successful treatment outcome of the underlying  
disease. In this regard, it is noteworthy that 44% of care-  
givers in the current study had correctly identified con-  
vulsions as a possible complication of fever. This is  
7
8
14  
Tessler, Taveras, and Kramer.  
It is pertinent to observe that the use of touch to assess  
fever has been an age old tradition passed down by par-  
ents, and indeed grandparents to the younger generation.  
Indeed it is a practice that is common in most communi-  
ties, and to a large extent, considered fairly useful. The  
above statement is supported by the finding in this study  
that 81.8% of caregivers would use touch alone to detect  
fever in their children, and 58.0% of caregivers had  
obtained this knowledge from friends and family. How-  
ever, in view of its suboptimal specificity, the usefulness  
14  
similar to 48% reported by Kramer. Th10is was higher,  
however, than 32% reported by Crocetti, and 15% re-  
5
ported by Schmitt. With regard to other potential harm  
emanating from fever in children, a large proportion of  
caregivers also identified brain damage, dehydration and  
death as possible complications. In the present study,  
2
7.5% of caregivers identified brain damage as a possi-  
ble complication of fever, an observation that is similar  
2
74  
1
4
to those of Kramer in America, in which 27% of par-  
ents felt brain damage could result from a fever.  
Whereas the fear of brain damage remains a common  
concern of parents and practitioners, this complication  
had rarely been shown to be8 caused by fever (even a  
regarding the home management of fever, presentation  
at a health facility, and the eventual outcome of febrile  
illnesses. This strengthens the need for increasing  
knowledge of parents regarding the import of fever and  
its appropriate management.  
1
temperature as high as 42°C). Instead, it is the  
underlying cause of the febrile illness, rather than the  
fever itself that is lar5g,1e9ly responsible for this morbidity  
Authors’ Contributions  
MBA conceptualized the work, coordinated the study,  
collected and analysed the data, and prepared the manu-  
script. WBRJ participated in the conceptualization,  
analysis and supervision of the study, and the prepara-  
tion of the final manuscript. All authors read and  
approved the final manuscript.  
(
i.e. brain damage).  
Furthermore, in the present  
study, 20% and 43% of caregivers respectively, identi-  
fied dehydration and death as the principal dangers of  
fever. These values were significantly higher than the  
percentages r1e4corded earlier by Kramer (4% and 11%  
respectively).  
Conflict of interests: None  
Funding: None  
Conclusion  
Acknowledgement  
Caregivers in the study population had a poor knowl-  
edge of fever, especially regarding its method of detec-  
tion and possible complications in children. This knowl-  
edge gap is likely to impact negatively on decisions  
We appreciate the contributions of the paediatric  
residents and nurses that assisted in recruitment of the  
subjects and data collection.  
References  
1
2
3
4
.
.
.
.
Mackowiak PA. History of clinical  
thermometry. In: Mackowiak PA,  
ed. Fever: Basic mechanism and  
management. 2nd ed. Philadelphia:  
Lippincott-Raven, 1995:1-10.  
Eskerud JR, Laerum E, Fagerthun  
H, Lunde PKM, Naess A. Fever in  
general practice. Frequency and  
diagnoses. Fam Pract 1992; 9:263  
8. Taveras EM, Durousseau S, Flores  
15. Nwanyanwu OC, Ziba C, Redd C,  
Luby SP. Palpation as a method of  
fever determination in Malawian  
children who are less than 5 years  
old: how reliable is it? Ann Trop  
Med Parasitol 1997;91(4):359-64.  
16. Akinbami FO, Orimadegun AE,  
Tongo OO, Okafor OO, Akinyinka  
OO. Detection of fever in children  
emergency care: comparisons of  
tactile and rectal temperatures in  
Nigerian children. BMC Research  
Notes 2010; 3:108.  
17. Singh M, Pai M, Kalantri SP. Ac-  
curacy of perception and touch for  
detecting fever in adults: a hospital  
based study from a rural tertiary  
hospital in Central India. Tropical  
Medicine and International  
G. Parents’ beliefs and practices  
regarding childhood fever. Pediatr  
Emerg Care 2004; 20:579-87.  
9. Al-Eissa YA, Al-Aloba SA, Gazal  
SS, Al-Wakeel AS. Parental per-  
ceptions of fever in children. Ann  
Saudi Med 2000;20(3-4):202-05.  
10. Crocetti M, Moghbeli N, Serwint  
J. Fever phobia revisited: Have  
parental misconceptions about  
fever changed in the last 20 years?  
Pediatrics 2001; 107:1241-46.  
11. Tagbo BN, Ibeneme CA, Adimora  
GN, Ilechukwu CGA. Mothers’  
perception and management of  
childhood fever at the University  
of Nigeria Teaching Hospital,  
Enugu. J Coll of Med 2010; 15  
(1):44-50.  
-69.  
Krauss BS, Harakal T, Fleisher  
GR. The spectrum and frequency  
of illness presenting to a pediatric  
emergency department. Pediatr  
Emerg Care 1991;7(2):67-71.  
Nelson DS, Walsh K, Fleisher GR.  
Spectrum and frequency of pediat-  
ric illness presenting to a general  
community hospital emergency  
department. Pediatrics 1992; 90:5-  
Health; 8:408-414.  
1
0.  
12. Oyedeji GA. Socio-economic and  
cultural background of hospital-  
ized children in Ilesha. Nig J Paed.  
1985; 12: 111-7.  
13. Blumenthal I. What parents think  
of fever. Family practice 1998; 15  
(6):513-18.  
18. Bull JMC. Whole body hyperther-  
mia as an anticancer agent. CA-a  
cancer J Clin. 1982;32(2):123-8.  
19. Avner JR. Acute fever. Pediatr  
Rev 2009; 30:5-13.  
5
6
7
.
.
.
Schmitt BD. Fever phobia: Mis-  
conceptions of parents about fever.  
Am J Dis Child 1980;134:176-81.  
Adam D, Stankov G. Treatment of  
fever in childhood. Eur J Pediatr  
1
994;153:394-402.  
Tessler H. Unrealistic concerns  
about fever in children. The influ-  
ence of cultural, ethnic and Socio-  
demographic factors. IMAJ  
14. Kramer MS, Naimark L, Leduc  
DG. Parental fever phobia and its  
correlates. Pediatrics 1985;75:  
1110-3.  
2
008;10: 346-49.